Why hand gestures are important




















An example of an equivalence problem is shown here:. Students had to decide what number to put in the blank, to make the right side equal to the left. For example, in the above equation, the number seven belongs in the blank, so that both sides equal The teacher taught half the kids using just speech. All students were then tested on the equivalence problems twice: Posttest 1 was immediately after learning and Posttest 2 was 24 h later.

The students were also given a Transfer Test. The researchers wanted to determine whether the children could extend their knowledge of equivalence to other types of problems using similar rules. For example, third and fourth graders solved a multiplication equivalence problem such as:. Second graders, who were not quite ready for multiplication, solved more complicated addition problems on the Transfer Test, which did not have any of the same numbers on both sides, such as:.

The scientists compared the number of correct answers from children who saw gestures to those of the children who were taught with only speech. On Posttest 1, Posttest 2, and the Transfer Test, the students who saw gestures scored better than the students who only learned with speech. Figure 2 shows the difference between these two groups of children.

Furthermore, students who learned with gestures also performed significantly better on Posttest 2 than they had on Posttest 1. This showed that gestures not only helped students learn better but gestures also helped their performance get better over time. Students who learned with only speech did not perform better on Posttest 2 than they did on Posttest 1.

Not only do students learn better when their teacher gestures but they also learn better when they use gestures themselves. A group of scientists studied whether third and fourth graders learned math better when they gestured during class [ 3 ]. The teachers taught the students how to solve addition equivalence math problems like the ones above. After instruction, all students took a posttest that consisted of new addition equivalence problems.

Then, 4 weeks later, all students completed a follow-up test with similar types of questions. The follow-up test allowed the researchers to test if gestures helped learning over time. Students from all three conditions performed similarly on the posttest that they completed right after learning: making gestures when learning did not benefit learning immediately.

However, on the follow-up test 4 weeks later, the students who learned by only making gestures and the students who learned by making gestures along with speech performed better than the students who learned only by speech Figure 3. The researchers think that gesturing during learning helped the children produce stronger memories, because while gesturing they activated motor areas of the brain that were not activated in children who did not gesture.

We have now shown you that gestures can improve learning, both when children see other people using gestures and when the children themselves use gestures. Is there something about gestures that can help us know when children are ready to learn something new? This seems to be the case for babies learning language. Before babies can talk, they communicate by pointing. Between 9 and 12 months, babies begin pointing to request objects like a bottle or a toy. The use of these early gestures tends to predict the first words that babies say a few months later.

Babies usually say their first word at about 12 months. Before babies begin to combine multiple words together, they combine words and gestures. These combinations are called gesture-speech mismatches because the information in speech and gesture is different, or mismatched. In fact, these mismatches predict how well the babies will learn language: babies who produce more of these gesture-speech mismatches when they are 18 months old go on to produce more complex sentences when they are 3 years old!

In these cases, gestures tell us what the child already knows and help us predict when the child will learn new words. Researchers were curious about whether gestures predicted learning in school-age children as well. The researchers studied the gestures children made when explaining how they solved mathematical equivalence problems, like the ones described earlier [ 5 ].

The researchers thought that, even though children might not be able to communicate how to solve these problems using speech, they might express some knowledge of equivalence in their gestures.

However, it might be evident from their gestures that the children are beginning to understand that the two sides of the equation are separate units. For example, the student might sweep his or her hand first under the left side of the equation and then under the right side, like the teachers did in the experiment above. The researchers wondered whether gesturing toward each side of the equal sign separately was signaling that the child was beginning to understand the concept of equivalence but could not yet form the idea into words.

The effects of elimination of hand gestures and of verbal codability on speech performance. Graziano, M. When speech stops, gesture stops: evidence from developmental and crosslinguistic comparisons. Hadar, U. Ideational gestures and speech in brain-damaged subjects. Iconic gestures, imagery, and word retrieval in speech. Semiotica , — Hallowell, B. Hauk, O. Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex.

Neuron 41, — Hayes, J. Traumatic brain injury as a disorder of brain connectivity. Henke, K. A model for memory systems based on processing modes rather than consciousness. Herrmann, M. Nonverbal communication as a compensative strategy for severely nonfluent aphasics? Hilliard, C. Bridging gaps in common ground: speakers design their gestures for their listeners. Hilverman, C. Gesture height reflects common ground status even in patients with amnesia.

Patients with hippocampal amnesia successfully integrate gesture and speech. Hand gestures support word learning in patients with hippocampal amnesia. Hippocampus 28, — Hippocampal declarative memory supports gesture production: evidence from amnesia. Cortex 85, 25— The influence of the hippocampus and declarative memory on word use: patients with amnesia use less imageable words.

Hoetjes, M. Reduction in gesture during the production of repeated references. Hogrefe, K. Co-speech hand movements during narrations: What is the impact of right vs.

Neuropsychologia 93, — Non-verbal communication in severe aphasia: Influence of aphasia, apraxia, or semantic processing?

Comprehensibility and neural substrate of communicative gestures in severe aphasia. Holler, J. Communicating common ground: how mutually shared knowledge influences speech and gesture in a narrative task. Hostetter, A. When do gestures communicate?

A meta-analysis. Gesture 7, 73— Visible embodiment: gestures as simulated action. Language, gesture, action! A test of the Gesture as Simulated Action framework. Gesture as simulated action: revisiting the framework. The effect of visual vs. Hough, M. Word retrieval failure episodes after traumatic brain injury.

Aphasiology 22, — Descriptive discourse abilities of traumatic brain-injured adults. Aphasiology 17, — Hyman, B. Neurology 40, — Iverson, J. Gesture in children blind from birth.

Why people gesture when they speak. Nature , — Gesture paves the way for language development. Jacobs, N. The role of conversational hand gestures in a narrative task. Johnson, M. An enhanced protocol for constraint-induced aphasia therapy II: a case series. Jones, C. Selling the story: narratives and charisma in adults with TBI. Jones, G.

Phonological blocking in the tip of the tongue state. Cognition 26, — Kagan, A. Kaplan, J. Visuospatial deficits after right hemisphere stroke. Kelly, G.

Challenging behaviour profiles of people with acquired brain injury living in community settings. Kelly, S. Offering a hand to pragmatic understanding: the role of speech and gesture in comprehension and memory.

Kendon, A. Kiefer, M. Conceptual representations in mind and brain: theoretical developments, current evidence and future directions. Kim, M. The use of gesture following traumatic brain injury: a preliminary analysis. Aphasiology 29, — King, K. Mild traumatic brain injury: effects on naming in word retrieval and discourse. Kirk, E. Kirmess, M. Constraint induced language therapy in early aphasia rehabilitation. Aphasiology 24, — Kita, S. How do gestures influence thinking and speaking?

The gesture-for-conceptualization hypothesis. Competing conceptual representations trigger co-speech representational gestures. What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal?

Klooster, N. Gestures make memories, but what kind? Patients with impaired procedural memory display disruptions in gesture production and comprehension. Kong, A. A comparison of coverbal gesture use in oral discourse among speakers with fluent and nonfluent aphasia. Co-verbal gestures among speakers with aphasia: influence of aphasia severity, linguistic and semantic skills, and hemiplegia on gesture employment in oral discourse. Kramer, J. Krauss, R. Why do we gesture when we speak? Kroenke, K.

Lexical learning in mild aphasia: gesture benefit depends on patholinguistic profile and lesion pattern. Cortex 49, — Learning by doing? The effect of gestures on implicit retrieval of newly acquired words. Kurczek, J. Differential contributions of hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex to self-projection and self-referential processing.

Neuropsychologia 73, — Laakso, M. Neural Transm. Parkinsons Dis. Landry, S. Social competence in young children with inflicted traumatic brain injury. Lanyon, L. Do the hands have it? The facilitation effects of arm and hand gesture on word retrieval in aphasia.

Aphasiology 23, — Leh, S. Microstructural integrity of hippocampal subregions is impaired after mild traumatic brain injury. Neurotrauma 34, — Levelt, W.

Speaking: From Intention to Articulation. Li, Y. PLoS One e Macedonia, M. Bringing back the body into the mind: gestures enhance word learning in foreign language. Gestures enhance foreign language learning. Biolinguistics 6, — Maher, L. A pilot study of use-dependent learning in the context of Constraint Induced Language Therapy. Management of anomia. Stroke Rehabil. Marstaller, L. Individual differences in the gesture effect on working memory. Martin, I. Evaluating the causes of impaired irony comprehension following traumatic brain injury.

Aphasiology 19, — Martin, N. Martin, P. Theory of mind deficits in patients with acquired brain injury: a quantitative review. Neuropsychologia 48, — Mayberry, R. McDonald, S. Pragmatic language skills after closed head injury: ability to meet the informational needs of the listener. Social perception deficits after traumatic brain injury: interaction between emotion recognition, mentalizing ability, and social communication.

Neuropsychology 18, — Pragmatic language skills after closed head injury: ability to negotiate requests. McNeill, D. Gesture and Thought. Cienki, E. Fricke, S. Ladewig, D. McNeill, and S. Tessendorf Berlin: De Gruyter Moutin , — Communicative effects of speech-mismatched gestures. Meinzer, M. Recovery from aphasia as a function of language therapy in an early bilingual patient demonstrated by fMRI.

Neuropsychologia 45, — Intensive language training in the rehabilitation of chronic aphasia: efficient training by laypersons. Melinger, A. Conceptualisation load triggers gesture production. Gesture and the communicative intention of the speaker. Mol, L. Miyake, D. Peebles, and R. Cooper Sapporo: Cognitive Science Society , — Seeing and being seen: the effects on gesture production.

Morrel-Samuels, P. Word familiarity predicts temporal asynchrony of hand gestures and speech. Morsella, E. The role of gestures in spatial working memory and speech. Mueller, K. Murray, L. Attention and other cognitive deficits in aphasia: presence and relation to language and communication measures. Murteira, A. Can gesture observation help people with aphasia name actions? Cortex , 86— Taking action in hand: effects of gesture observation on action verb naming. Mutlu, B. Social-cue perception and mentalizing ability following traumatic brain injury: a human-robot interaction study.

Nickels, L. Constraint Induced Aphasia Therapy: volunteer-led, unconstrained and less intense delivery can be effective. Neurorehabilitation 39, 97— Obermeier, C. The benefit of gestures during communication: evidence from hearing and hearing-impaired individuals. Gesturing with an injured brain: how gesture helps children with early brain injury learn linguistic constructions.

Child Lang. Perry, M. Transitional knowledge in the acquisition of concepts. Pierce, J. Constraint and multimodal approaches to therapy for chronic aphasia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Pine, K. More gestures than answers: children learning about balance. Ping, R. PsyArXiv [Preprint]. Gesturing saves cognitive resources when talking about nonpresent objects.

Prutting, C. A clinical appraisal of the pragmatic aspects of language. Speech Hear. Functional links between motor and language systems. Constraint-induced therapy of chronic aphasia after Stroke.

Stroke 32, — Radice-Neumann, D. Overview of impaired facial affect recognition in persons with traumatic brain injury.

Rauscher, F. Gesture, speech, and lexical access: the role of lexical movements in speech production. Rigon, A. Procedural memory following moderate-severe traumatic brain injury: group performance and individual differences on the rotary pursuit task. Spatial relational memory in individuals with traumatic brain injury. Relationship between individual differences in functional connectivity and facial-emotion recognition abilities in adults with traumatic brain injury.

Neuroimage Clin. Different aspects of facial affect recognition impairment following traumatic brain injury: the role of perceptual and interpretative abilities. Rodriguez, A. Aphasiology 20, — Rose, M. The differential facilitatory effects of gesture and visualisation processes on object naming in aphasia. Aphasiology 15, — The effects of semantic and gesture treatments on verb retrieval and verb use in aphasia. The utility of arm and hand gestures in the treatment of aphasia.

Releasing the constraints on aphasia therapy: the positive impact of gesture and multimodality treatments. Multi-modality aphasia therapy is as efficacious as a constraint-induced aphasia therapy for chronic aphasia: a phase 1 study. Constraint-induced or multi-modal personalized aphasia rehabilitation COMPARE : a randomized controlled trial for stroke-related chronic aphasia. Stroke 14, — Communicative effectiveness of pantomime gesture in people with aphasia.

A systematic review of gesture treatments for post-stroke aphasia. Ross, E. Dominant language functions of the right hemisphere? Rousseaux, M. An analysis of communication in conversation after severe traumatic brain injury. Rowe, M. Differences in early gesture explain SES disparities in child vocabulary size at school entry. Early gesture selectively predicts later language learning. First Lang. Sainson, C. Communication disorders and executive function impairment after severe traumatic brain injury: an exploratory study using the GALI a grid for linguistic analysis of free conversational interchange.

Sauer, E. Early gesture predicts language delay in children with pre- or perinatal brain lesions. Schiaratura, L. Sekine, K. The relationship of aphasia type and gesture production in people with aphasia. Gesture production patterns in aphasic discourse: in-depth description and preliminary predictions. Singer, M. Small, S. Smith, W. So, W. Producing gestures facilitates route learning. PLoS One 9:e Sparks, R. Aphasia rehabilitation resulting from melodic intonation therapy.

Cortex 10, — Stuss, D. Traumatic brain injury: relation to executive dysfunction and the frontal lobes. Sueyoshi, A. The role of gestures and facial cues in second language listening comprehension. Sweller, N. The effects of observing and producing gestures on Japanese word learning. Acta Psychol. Taub, E. Movement in nonhuman primates deprived of somatosensory feedback.

Sport Sci. Technique to improve chronic motor deficit after stroke. The functional significance of cortical reorganization and the parallel development of CI therapy. Theakston, A. Handling agents and patients: representational cospeech gestures help children comprehend complex syntactic constructions. Turkstra, L. Looking while listening and speaking. Assessment of pragmatic communication skills in adolescents after traumatic brain injury.

Ullman, M. A neural dissociation within language: evidence that the mental dictionary is part of declarative memory, and that grammatical rules are processed by the procedural system. Valenzeno, L. Part of the message comes in gesture: how people with aphasia convey information in different gesture types as compared with information in their speech.

Aphasiology 31, — Wagner, P. Gesture and speech in interaction: an overview. Speech Commun. Wagner, S. Probing the mental representation of gesture: Is handwaving spatial?

Wehman, P. Critical factors associated with the successful supported employment placement of patients with severe traumatic brain injury.

Wei, C. Wilssens, I. Constraint-induced aphasia therapy versus intensive semantic treatment in fluent aphasia. Wolf, S. Effect of constraint-induced movement therapy on upper extremity function 3 to 9 months after stroke: the EXCITE randomized clinical trial.

JAMA , — Retention of upper limb function in stroke survivors who have received constraint-induced movement therapy: the EXCITE randomised trial. Lancet Neurol. Zwaan, R. Pecher, and R. Zwaan Cambridge: Cambridge University Press , — The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author s and the copyright owner s are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice.

No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. Introduction When people talk, they move their hands. Theoretical Underpinnings of Speech and Gesture There has been much theoretical interest in describing the relationship between speech and gesture.

Gesture for Communication Like the study of spoken language, which can be characterized by its parts e. Gesture for the Listener Perhaps the most obvious communication benefits of gesture are those produced for the listener.

Gesture for the Speaker While it may seem intuitive that gesture has functions for the listener, gesture also has important benefits for the speaker. Gesture for Cognition Unlike speech, the spontaneous gestures that speakers produce have no standardized form, but rather, are idiosyncratic.

Gesture Reduces Cognitive Load Given that speakers gesture more when a task is cognitively or linguistically complex Melinger and Kita, ; Kita and Davies, , it is critical to understand how gesture confers cognitive benefits. Spontaneous Gestures Predict Readiness to Learn Our hands not only reveal what we know but also what we are about to know. People will want to hear your second, third, etc.

This is a powerful persuasion technique. This is when you hold your hands in the steeple, but lower. And instead of your fingers pointing up, they point more forward.

Kennedy, Khrushchev, This is a great gesture you can use in a one-on-one conversation. Kennedy was famous first for using this. An emphatic, it does not exhibit the anger of the clenched fist or pointing finger, and so is thought to be less threatening. Kennedy, who can be seen using it in many speeches and images from his political career.

You can emphasize this by assuming a thinking pose. Like this one, by putting your finger to your chin and looking up slightly. First, figure out WHAT you want to say. It could be on a big stage at a conference.

At your sisters wedding. Or in a meeting at work. So, slightly exaggerated gestures are okay. Practice exaggerated gesturing in private. However, experts agree …. You see the problem, right? So, I have my go-to gestures now. Before you start using any hand gesture, be aware that some gestures have different meanings in different places.

For example …. English speaking countries usually count with the index finger as the first digit. Many European countries start counting with the thumb as the first digit. But there is no word or links to your sites that you have build.



0コメント

  • 1000 / 1000